Altman, DG and Bland, JM (2009). Parametric v non-parametric methods for data analysis. BMJ 338. doi: 10.1136/bmj.a3167

van der Worp, HB, et al. (2010). Can animal models of disease reliably inform human studies? PLOS Med 7(3):e1000245. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000245

Tuyttens, FAM, et al. (2014). Observer bias in animal behaviour research: can we believe what we score, if we score what we believe? Animal Behaviour 90:273-280. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.02.007

Schulz, KF and Grimes, DA (2002). Allocation concealment in randomised trials: defending against deciphering. Lancet 359(9306):614-8. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(02)07750-4

Schulz, KF, et al. (1995). Empirical evidence of bias. Dimensions of methodological quality associated with estimates of treatment effects in controlled trials. JAMA 273(5):408-12. doi: 10.1001/jama.273.5.408

Schulz, KF, Altman, DG and Moher, D (2002). Allocation concealment in clinical trials. JAMA 288(19):2406-2406. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.19.2406-JLT1120-4-1

Rosenthal, R and Fode, KL (1963). The effect of experimenter bias on the performance of the albino rat. Behavioral Science 8(3):183-189. doi: 10.1002/bs.3830080302

Polit, DF (2011). Blinding during the analysis of research data. Int J Nurs Stud 48(5):636-41. doi: 10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2011.02.010

Moher, D, et al. (1998). Does quality of reports of randomised trials affect estimates of intervention efficacy reported in meta-analyses? The Lancet 352(9128):609-613. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(98)01085-X

Macleod, MR, et al. (2009). Good laboratory practice: preventing introduction of bias at the bench. Stroke 40(3):e50-2. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.525386

Subscribe to